What is language?

 

Language is a learned human behavior.

All human societies have language.

Languages share certain universal traits.

The relationships between sounds and meaning are basically arbitrary.

Language uses a finite set of elements to create an infinite set of possible sentences.

Language consists of sounds, forms and meaning.

People use language for a wide variety of purposes.

Language use involves many psychological and emotional factors.

Linguistic knowledge includes knowledge of sounds, forms and meaning.

Language ability is innate in humans.

Language ability is common to individuals with vastly different levels of intelligence.

Language abilities are related to specific areas of the brain.

Aphasia studies show how particular areas of the brain relate to language functions.

 

Regardless of how primitive or abbreviated language may be, it is pivotal to

cognition: by means of it we designate numbers, perform mathematics, calculations, analyze perceptions, distinguish the essential from the nonessential, and form categories of distinct impressions. Apart from being a means of communicating, language is fundamental to perception and memory, thinking and behavior. It organizes our inner life.

 

A definition of language

 

There are numerous definitions of language. David Crystal defines language as

"The systematic, conventional use of sounds, signs or written symbols in a human society

for communication and self-expression."

 

Characteristics of human language

 

Lyons presents four major distinctives of human language:

1. Arbitrariness 2. Duality 3. Discreteness 4. Productivity

 

Language is arbitrary in terms of how certain sounds represent certain meanings in a

society. Except for onomatopoeia (and even these forms are culturally accepted

approximations of the real world sounds they represent), most words are simply a set of

sounds used as a symbol to represent an agreed upon meaning. The grammatical

structures of language are also arbitrary in that there is no single pattern for clauses or

noun phrases, etc. that all languages must follow. (Later, we will look at language

universals, which show some limits to this general statement.)

 

The duality of language refers to the fact that every 'piece' or unit of language fits into

larger 'pieces' or units. Sounds fit into syllables, syllables into words, words into phrases,

phrases into clauses, etc. We might also think of this as hierarchical.

 

 

Discreteness in language summarizes the fact that linguistic units are not constantly

changing; there are limits within in which change occurs. Thus, we can identify the

sounds, the syllable types, the clause types, etc. The number of total patterns is both

limited and identifiable.

Pinker notes: Non-human communication systems are based on one of three designs: a finite repertory of calls...a continuous analog signal that registers magnitude of some

state... or a set of random variations on a theme.... human language has a very different design. The combinatorial system called 'grammar' makes human language infinite... digital... and compositional....

 

Human language is also productive: speakers can always add (within limits) more words

or phrases or substitute synonymous forms in a sentence to make a "first time in the

history of the universe" statement.

The principle underlying grammar is unusual in the natural world. A grammar is

an example of a 'discrete combinatorial system.' A finite number of discrete

elements (in this case, words) are sampled, combined, and permuted to create

larger structures (in this case, sentences) with properties that are quite distinct

from those of their elements.

 

Locating language in the brain

 

Neurolinguistic studies have shown that there are a number of areas in the brain which

are used primarily for producing and comprehending language. By studying how damage

or injury to the brain (aphasia) affects language behavior, researchers have found that

language is primarily a function of the left hemisphere.

 

Two main types of aphasia suggest that language may be organized in terms of a lexical

and a grammatical part.

 

Broca's aphasia 'is characterized by labored speech, word-finding pauses, loss of

'function' words (grammatical morphemes), and, quite often, disturbed word order.'

 

Wiernicke's aphasia is characterized by semantic and lexical difficulties such as word

substitutions, difficulty comprehending speech, etc.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

How language functions

Language is used in many ways. Nida discusses eight ways that language is used:

 

Aesthetic - using language in aesthetic ways (e.g. poetry, advertising)

Cognitive - using words to think

Emotive - influencing the emotive state of the receptors

Expressive - expressing emotions

Informative - conveying information

Imperative - influencing behavior (stimulating action)

Interpersonal - making and maintaining relationships

Performative - modifying the state of the receptors

 

We might also add several other functions of language:

Documentary - recording information

Identificational - expressing one's identity

Interrogative - obtaining information

 

Words can do wonderful things. They sound purr. They can urge, they can

wheedle, whip, whine. They can sing, sass, singe. They can churn, check,

channelize. They can be a hup, 2, 3, 4. They can forge a fiery army out of a

hundred languid men. (Gwendolyn Brooks, afterword, Contending Forces, 1968)

 

Speech is power; speech is to persuade, to convert, to compel. (Ralph Waldo Emerson)

 

Models of language

 

The following chart adapted from Webb (1986:51) illustrates six factors that precede any

linguistic message:

 

A. Psychological factors: intentions of the speaker, and the speaker's opinions, attitudes, etc.

+ B. Social factors: social properties of the speaker (and hearer[s]). The traditional role of

speech in the society, the social roles of the participants, the social setting, etc.

+ C. Situational factors: e.g. the physical setting

 

ARE PROCESSED THROUGH THE

 

D. The total set of linguistics resources available to the speaker

+ E. Rhetorical devices: discourse genres, conversational routines, strategies, forms and norms

+ F. Nonlinguistic communicative resources: gestures, facial expressions, body language, clothes

 

TO CREATE

 

G. The Text or linguistic message

Language acquisition

 

Children acquire language in stages and these stages appear to be universal.

The ability of children to acquire language is not generally dependent on race, social

class, geography, or intelligence.

At some point the ability to acquire language decreases.

Adults can learn additional languages given sufficient opportunity.

Adults learn in different ways than children.

Adult language learners benefit from using their preferred learning style(s).

By imitating speech a subject is more easily learned than by study and books.

 

Definitions

Many linguists distinguish language acquisition and language learning; children acquire

their first language, after that people learn additional languages.

Children around the world develop linguistic knowledge in similar ways and at similar

stages of their life, no matter how 'difficult' the language may appear.

 

Pinker notes "...acquisition of a normal language is guaranteed for children up to the age

of six, is steadily compromised from then until shortly after puberty, and is rare

thereafter."

 

Researchers have concluded that some aspects of language are innate in humans; human

beings are predisposed to learn and use language. The innate form of language contains a

universal grammar that is later shaped by the particular forms of the language(s) a child

is exposed to. Aitchinson outlines the characteristics of biologically controlled

behaviors such as language as follows:

· Language emerges before it is "necessary".

· Language is not the result of a conscious decision.

· Language is not triggered by external events.

· Language is not the result of direct teaching or intensive practice.

· Language develops along standard lines correlated with age and other types of development.

· Language is acquired during a "critical period".

 

How do infants acquire their first language?

1. Not by imitation

2. Not by reinforcement

3. Not by memorizing grammar rules

4. In stages

5. By assimilating rules

 

I have...discovered by observation how I learned to speak. I did not learn by

elders teaching me words in any systematic way, as I was soon after taught to

read and write. But of my own motion...I strove with cries and various sounds and

much moving of my limbs to utter the feelings of my heart—all this in order to get

my own way.

Stages of language development

pre-linguistic sounds

early linguistic sounds

babbling

holophrastic phrases

two-word phrases

telegraphic speech

 

Cross-language studies show that infants start babbling with certain sounds, no matter

what language is spoken around them. Among the first consonants made by infants

around the world are (Data from O'Grady, et. al. 1994:364):

p t k

b d g

m n

w j h

s

 

How children learn the meaning of words

 

Over generalization

'Daddy' as any adult male

 

Under generalization

'doggie' as just the family pet

 

The critical age hypothesis

Pinker presents many aspects of the critical age hypothesis. In summary, he says:

Thus, language acquisition might be like other biological functions. The linguistic

clumsiness of tourists and students might be the price we pay for the linguistic

genius we displayed as babies, just as the decrepitude of age is the price we pay

for the vigor of youth.

 

Differences between children and adult learners

linguistic patterns

emotional makeup

situational variety

time

types of input

 

These natives are unintelligent—We can't understand their language.

(Chinweizu, "Colonizer's Logic" in Voices from Twentieth-Century Africa, 1988)

 

In Paris they simply stared when I spoke to them in French; I never did succeed

in making those idiots understand their language. (Mark Twain)

 

 

Paul Robeson spoke twenty-five languages not as an exercise but as a source

through which he could absorb the many cultures into which he had not been

born, but to which he was instinctively determined to belong. He consumed a

language for the cultural essence it contained and became in practice, in custom,

and in habit a loyal member of all the groups whose songs he sang.

 

Charles V said that a man who knew four languages was worth four men.

(Lord Macauley)

 

Language is the building block of consciousness. To accurately understand the

soul of a people, you not only search for their outward manifestations…but you

examine their language. (Haki Madhubuti)

 

Knowing your own learning style(s)

visual

auditory

tactile

 

Adapting your language learning to your best learning patterns

writing notes

interactive activities

active participation

 

He that travelleth into a country before he hath some entrance into the language,

goeth to school, and not to travel. (Francis Bacon)

 

 

The Beginnings of Literacy        Joan Brooks McLane and Gillian Dowley McNamee

                                                    Erikson Institute

 

       Literacy often begins early, long before children encounter formal school instruction in writing and reading. Literacy itself is not easy to define, and there are many disputes and unresolved questions about how literacy develops. Central to many recent discussions of literacy is the notion that writing and reading are ways of making, interpreting, and communicating meaning. Reading is defined as the ability to "take meaning from print," (Heath, 1982) and writing as the ability to use print to communicate with others. According to these definitions, reading and writing are more than simply decoding and encoding print: they are ways of constructing and conveying meaning with written language. Becoming literate, then, is a multifaceted phenomenon that involves more than learning a set of technical skills (such as learning the alphabet, learning how to form letters and spell words, and learning how to decode print) that are typically taught in elementary school; becoming literate also includes mastering a complex set of understandings, attitudes, expectations and behaviors, as well as specific skills, related to written language (Erickson, 1984; Cook-Gumperz, 1986).

 

       Many young children begin to learn about writing and reading well before they start elementary school. However, their early literacy activities may look quite different from more mature, conventional forms of writing and reading. For example, one weekend, Jennifer, who had just turned two, went with her father to visit her grandmother in the country. Her grandmother read her Johnny Lion's Book by Edith Thacher Hurd-- a story book about a young lion who dreams of going hunting in the woods. About an hour later, Jennifer, who had been playing outside, called to her father, "I read a book, Daddy." She had picked two big leaves which she now held, one lying flat on the palm of each hand. She and her father sat down on the steps, side by side, and Jennifer started "reading" her "book" in what her grandmother described as a "dramatic voice," which sounded very much like reading: "And a big bear (pause) went into the woods and she chased a big lion (pause) and she caught a big lion." After another pause, her father said, "and then what did the bear do?" Jennifer answered, "Then the big bear went home to her mommy."

 

       Jennifer's pretend reading makes it clear that she is interested in reading and stories. She does not yet know how to read (or write) in a conventional sense, but she pretends to know. Playing with reading is one way she learns about written language and how it can be used. This incident also shows how familiar Jennifer already is with reading--two leaves can serve as her "text" and she can invent a simple but coherent story and tell it in a voice that accurately mimics a reading intonation. And it indicates that Jennifer finds reading interesting and pleasurable--as well as a good way to capture her father's attention.

 

       There are many ways that children make connections with writing and reading, and many pathways into literacy. Writing and reading can enter young children's lives in a variety of ways. Early experiences with literacy may be initiated by the child or by other people, they may be playful or work-like, and may take place at home, in the neighborhood or in community settings such as preschools, daycare centers, and churches. Early literacy experiences can include pretending to write and read stories and poems, writing a thank-you letter to a distant grandmother, receiving instruction in how to form the letters of one's name, listening to a story being read aloud, or reading passages from the Bible. The range and diversity of early literacy experiences suggests that there are many ways that children make connections with writing and reading, and many pathways to literacy.

 

       Bridges to literacy

 

       Literacy development often starts in young children's early symbol using activities: in talking, in play and fantasy, in scribbling and drawing, in pretend reading and writing. Between the ages of 1-5 children learn to use symbols they invent for themselves and those "donated by the culture" (Gardner & Wolf, 1979, p.vii). The use of symbols--which may include words, gestures, marks on paper, objects modeled in clay, and so forth--makes it possible to represent experience, feelings and ideas. Symbols also allow children to go beyond the immediate here and now and to create imaginary worlds. This is what they do when they talk about storybook plots, when they make up stories, engage in pretend play, or draw images on paper--and later when they read books and write stories. As children begin to experiment with writing and reading, often in playful ways, they may find they can use these new symbolic modes in some of the same ways they used earlier developed symbolic forms--so that talking, drawing, and playing can serve as "bridges" to literacy, as children discover that writing and reading offer them new and interesting resources for constructing and communicating meaning (Gundlach, 1982; Dyson, 1986; Vygotsky, 1978).

 

       Play: making connections with writing and reading

 

       Play consumes much of young children's time and energy, and for many children, play is where writing and reading begin. Play is the arena in which young children make connections between their immediate personal world and activities that are important in the larger social world of family and community, and play is the context in which many children find ways to make culturally valued activities part of their own personal experience. When children play with writing and reading, they are actively trying to use--and to understand and make sense of --reading and writing long before they can actually read and write. When books, paper, and writing material are among the objects children play with, important literacy learning can occur. As they experiment with written language, often in playful ways, children begin to learn what writing and reading are, and what they can do with them. At the same time, children can acquire a range of information and skills related to writing and reading, as well as feelings and expectations about themselves as potential readers and writers. This multifaceted body of knowledge and attitudes constitutes early or "emergent" literacy (Holdaway, 1979; Teale & Sulzby, 1986).

 

       Play appears to have at least two potential links to the development of literacy: First, as a symbolic activity, pretend play allows children to develop and refine their capacities to use symbols, to represent experience, and to construct imaginary worlds, capacities they will draw on when they begin to write and read. Second, as an orientation or approach to experience, play can make the various roles and activities of people who read and write more meaningful and hence more accessible to young children.

 

       When children create imaginary situations in pretend play, they invent and inhabit "alternative" or "possible worlds." This is similar to what they do when they listen to storybooks, and to what they do when they read or write stories themselves. Indeed, there are similarities between pretend play and storytelling, and in the kinds of competence the two require (Britton, 1983; Bruner, 1984; Galda, 1984). Many children make up their first stories in the context of pretend play, creating and enacting their own dramatic narratives (and reenacting favorite stories they have heard being read aloud). Indeed, one of the things that attracts young children to pretend play is the chance to tell stories. Later, many children are attracted to writing and reading for the same reasons: they find they can participate in stories told by others.

 

       As children mature, their pretend play and the symbolic transformations they use to create and sustain it become increasingly elaborate, complex, and abstract. With development, pretend play becomes less dependent on physical props, gestures, and actions, and relies increasingly on ideas, imagination, and language. Children often employ abundant, rich language in pretend play. An increasing proportion of the time devoted to pretend play is spent in talk, as children discuss the setting, the characters or roles, and the plots they will enact in their play. Indeed, at times it seems as though "the saying is the playing (Garvey Berndt, 1975, p.9)." As pretend play becomes increasingly dependent on language to create possible worlds and to express and communicate meanings, it comes closer to the experiences of storytelling, writing, and reading.

 

       Children seem to be able to play with almost anything: objects, movements, behaviors, roles, rules, and language. Many play with the implements and materials of written language: with paper, pencils, markers, crayons, and books, with the activities of writing and reading, and with the roles of writer and reader. Children incorporate both real and pretend writing and reading into their dramatic play, using them to enhance the drama and realism of the pretend situation. They may use characters and plots from their favorite storybooks. Some preschool children explore the tools and activities of literacy in playful ways, using pretend writing to create "stories," "poems," or "news bulletins"--as 4-and-a-half year-old Joshua did one Sunday afternoon: After dressing up in various costumes and pretending he was "a baby learning to fly," Joshua wrote some letters and letter like figures on a piece of paper. He showed this to this mother and said "Now wait for the surprise." Then he held the paper in front of his face while he shouted: "Good evening ladies and gentlemen! This is the ABC News! Now we have lots of weather!"

Joshua's written news report and his reading of it contain elements of real literacy--letter like shapes, and the understanding that these carry a message. In this sort of play children practice and integrate what they know or what they surmise about a range of activities and roles outside their immediate experience and understanding. As Greta Fein observed, "pretense [may] provide special opportunities for the partially understood and the dimly grasped to become more firmly mastered  (1979, p.206)." By playing with writing and reading, children become familiar with the tools of literacy and begin to learn how to use and control them. Such play also allows children to acquire some global notions of what writing and reading consist of and what they can do with them.

 

       In play the focus is on exploring rather than on accomplishing predetermined ends or goals, so there are few pressures to produce correct answers or final products. Play's nonliteral, not-for-real, "not-for-profit" orientation allows players the freedom to manipulate materials, experiences, roles and ideas in new, creative, experimental, "as if" ways (Bruner, 1977, p.v; Garvey, 1974). Play thus creates a risk-free context in which children do not have to worry about "getting it right" or about "messing up." This freedom may lead children to discover or invent possibilities--new ways of doing things and new ways of thinking about ideas--which may, in turn, lead them to new questions, problems, and solutions. Approaching writing and reading with such an experimental, "as if" attitude may help children realize that written language is something they can manipulate in a variety of ways and for a variety of purposes. "Playing at writing and reading--by scribbling, drawing, pretending to write, or pretending to read--may serve to open up the activities of writing and reading for children's consideration and exploration (Bruner, 1976; Sutton-Smith, 1979).

 

       Pretend play often involves reversals of everyday, real-life roles and power relationships. When they pretend, children can enact powerful roles such as mother, father, doctor, fireman, witch, monster, writer, and reader and take on the competencies that come with these roles, as Joshua did when he "wrote" and "read" the "ABC News". In doing this, Joshua assumed the confidence and power of someone who can write and read, and engaged in writing and reading on his own terms, defining them as he was able to carry them out at the moment.

 

       Play thus encourages children to act as if they are already competent in and about to control the activity under consideration: they can act as if they know how to cook, put out fires, kill monsters, read books, or write stories. Playing with the roles of writer and reader can give children a sense of ownership of these roles. Through play, children may come to feel that they are writers and readers long before they actually have the necessary skills and knowledge to write and read. The feelings of competence and control that can be seen in children such as Jennifer and Joshua are likely to nourish assumptions and expectations about becoming literate, and to give children the motivation to work at learning to write and read.

 

       Literacy and relationships

 

       While activities like talking, playing, and drawing are closely linked to writing and reading, and while their use often intertwine and overlap, there are no direct or inevitable transitions between earlier-and later-developed symbol systems. Whether and how children make connections between talking, playing, drawing, and writing and reading depends on the children's interests and personalities, on what is available and valued in their particular culture, on how the people around them use writing and reading in their own lives, and how these people initiate and respond to children's writing and reading activities.

 

       In other words, early literacy development does not simply happen; rather, it is part of a social process, embedded in children's relationships with parents, siblings, grandparents, friends, caretakers, and teachers. To understand the beginnings of literacy, one must study the environments in which young children develop, and the ways in which these settings provide opportunities for children to become involved with books, paper, and writing materials. Early experiences with literacy are part of the relationships, activities, and settings of young children's everyday lives. It is people who make writing and reading interesting and meaningful to young children. Family members, caretakers, and teachers play critical roles in early literacy development by serving as models, providing materials, demonstrating their use, offering help, instruction, and encouragement, and communicating hopes and expectations. To their interactions with young children, these people bring their own attitudes and expectations, both conscious and unconscious, about writing and reading, and about the child's eventual development as a writer and reader (Gundlach, McLane, Stott & McNamee, 1985).

 

       The beginning of writing

 

       Early writing activities tend to be more visible than early reading activities because they involve making something. If given crayons or pencils, children usually begin to scribble around the age of 18 months; they find scribbling interesting because it leaves a visible trace--they have made something that didn't exist before. When children encounter print in their environment, they use this visual information in their scribbling and pretend writing. Marie Clay (1975) has shown that as scribbling develops, it begins to incorporate various features of conventional written language, such as linearity, horizontally, and repetition. As children learn that marks and letters represent or stand for something, they are developing an understanding of what Clay calls the "sign concept"--which is of central importance in learning to write and read.

 

       Robert Gundlach (1982) has argued that beginning writers need to master the functions, uses and purposes of writing; the forms and features of written language; and the processes of writing. Children must learn what writing can do, and, in particular, what they can do with writing.

 

       The beginnings of reading

 

       Young children can begin to learn about the complex process of deriving meaning from print long before they can decode or even recognize letters. Susan Knontos has pointed out that "before they become readers, young children must learn why people read and what people do when they read (1986, p. 58)." As in learning about writing, young children begin to understand the enterprise of reading from observing and participating in activities with family members and other competent readers. When young children see other people reading, and when others read to them or involve them in other activities related to reading, they become familiar with print and some of its uses. For example, when they see people who are important to them reading a recipe to bake a cake, reading a newspaper to find out what movies are on TV, or reading letters from distant relatives in Vietnam, young children experience reading as a meaningful activity and part of everyday life. In addition, many adults point out print to children, helping them notice a particular configuration of letters such as the spelling of their name or the name of a favorite brand of breakfast cereal. In this way, young children may develop a "sight vocabulary" of words like "STOP," "Crest," and "McDonald's" (Heath, 1986, p.20; Harste, Woodward, & Burke, 1984).

 

       Reading books to children is a powerful way of introducing them to literacy, and it is the one early experience that has been identified as making a difference in later success in learning to read in school (Anderson, Hiebert, Scott & Wilkinson, 1985). But this is not usually the reason that adults, particularly parents, read to young children. More often, they do it because they find it enjoyable, they see that children enjoy it, and they may believe that reading nourishes children's minds and enriches their relationships. When adults read to children, the occasion tends to be warm and intimate; parents, caretakers, and older children often hold young children on their laps or sit close to them while reading aloud, and their attention is focused on their interaction with the child. The reader is usually sensitive to what interests the child, and what he or she finds scary, exciting or amusing. Reading often includes conversations about the characters in the book, about what they might be thinking and feeling, and about experiences in the child's own life that are related to those in the book (Lindfors, 1984; Deloache, 1984).

 

       As children are read to they acquire an enormous amount of information about reading and the world of books. They learn what books are, what you do with them, and how you talk about them (Snow & Ninio, 1986; Teale, 1982). They learn that

 written words can create imaginary worlds beyond the immediate here and now. They learn that written language has its own rhythms and conventions. They learn about specific features of written languages: for example, that the black marks on the page are letters and words, and that print goes from left to right and from top to bottom on the page. Gradually, children learn that the reader is reconstructing the story through the words written on the page--that print has a precise and unchanging meaning. And, perhaps most important, they come to expect that books will be interesting, challenging, exciting, and comforting, developing, in Holdaway's words, "high expectations of print" (1979).

 

       One way that children show us they are learning from being read to is through pretending to read storybooks by themselves. Don Holdaway was probably the first to point out that very young children who are read to frequently spend a great deal of time on their own with favorite storybooks, pretending to read them and reenacting the behaviors they observed while they were being read to. In observing a number of children between the ages of 2 and 5 "reading" favorite storybooks, Holdaway was struck by how hard the children worked to recapture the meaning of the stories: "They have remembered very little of the surface verbal level: what they have remembered most firmly is the meanings (Holdaway, l979, p. 44)." The children were not giving a memorized rendition of a story, but were, instead, working to construct the message of the story using the rhythms and sounds of language in which they first heard the message.

 

       Elizabeth Sulzby (l985) describes a progression of changes in children's pretend reading as they gradually approach independent reading. Preschoolers' reading of favorite books is, for the most part, guided by "reading" the pictures in the book. Young children hold the book and turn the page quite deliberately, while naming or commenting on what they see in the pictures. In time, and as they become more familiar with the story, they "read" the book by making up a story, creating a rough story line that follows the sequence of pictures. Gradually the language they use in "reading" (while still looking at the pictures), sounds more like real reading--the child's voice and intonation come to sound like written language read aloud.

 

       Children ages 3, 4 or 5 may give close renditions or even verbatim recitations of stories they have heard frequently. As Sulzby and Holdaway have observed, children do not simply memorize the text but work from a strong sense of what the story should sound like, and they work to retrieve and reconstruct the meaning of the text. Children strive to get the exact working--they sometimes hesitate, correct themselves, or ask others for help. Young children's independent efforts to read books demonstrate the wealth of knowledge about books, print and narrative they acquire while they are being read to.

 

       It is clear that over the months and years of being read to, children learn many of the subtle details of behavior and speaking that go with reading a book. Pretend reading allows children to role-play, to reenact and try out the behaviors, skills and thinking processes that are part of reading. This long period of play brings children very close to actual reading.

 

       Conclusion

 

       Early literacy development is closely tied to the specifics of young children's relationships and activities. To these relationships and activities, children bring their curiosity, their interest in communicating and interacting with others, and their inclination to be a part of family and community life. They also bring their desire to use and control materials and tools that they perceive as important to the people around them--their urge to "do it myself." And they bring their willingness to seek help from more proficient writers and readers. When they interact with more competent writers and readers, children serve as "spontaneous apprentices" (in George Miller's phrase), learning about written language and how to use and control it for a range of purposes.

 

       What is the relationship between early experiences with literacy and later, long-term literacy development? There are as yet no definitive answers to this question, but as in other aspects of psychological development, we assume that there is a relationship between early literacy experience and later mature literacy. How this relationship unfolds for a particular child will depend on several factors which interact with one another in complex ways. These include the child's interests, temperament and personality, opportunities at home and in the neighborhood for writing and reading, as well as the nature and quality of the instruction the child encounters in school.

 

Language and Society

 

Language and culture are very interconnected.

People typically adjust their way of speaking to different situations.

Language is often a powerful symbol of ethnic identity.

People make language choices for a number of reasons, including economic

advancement, a desire for education, intermarriage, etc.

A language consists of mutually intelligible dialects.

Dialects may be based on differences of geography, social class, gender, age, etc.

People have strong feelings about dialect and language differences.

People may have negative attitudes about languages around them.

People may have negative attitudes about their own language.

It is important to understand people's attitudes towards various types of language.

 

A living language is a throbbing, vital thing, ever changing, ever growing and

mirroring the people who speak it and write it. It has its roots in the masses,

though its superstructure may represent the culture of a few. (Jawaharlal Nehru)

 

It is impossible for an Englishman to open his mouth, without making some other

Englishman despise him. (G. B. Shaw, Pygmalion, Preface)

 

Definitions

Linguistics is the scientific study of language. The study of the formal patterns of

language is of interest to descriptive linguists and theoreticians. Language is more than

grammatical structures, however. The performance of individuals and the ways

individuals adjust their speech to different situations must also be studied. An

observationally adequate sociolinguistic study would describe the competence factors of

native speakers: those social factors which affect which lexical, phonological, and

grammatical forms they use to convey a particular idea in a particular setting.

Sociolinguistics can be defined in a number of ways. In general, sociolinguistics is the

study of how language is used within a society. Sociolinguistics can include the study of:

dialects,

attitudes toward language,

bilingualism,

language planning, etc.

The tools of phonetics and grammatical analysis still come into play, but other skills,

such as social science research methods, are at least as important to sociolinguistic

research.

Nida summarizes sociolinguistics as follows:

Sociolinguistics looks at language from the standpoint of its social context. In

other words, it is concerned about speakers, receptors, setting, content, form, and

the relation of language to other codes. The focus is not so much upon the

sentence or text but upon the speech event as such. In other words, this is

language in action. (Nida 1986:1)

Nida (1986:4) notes sixteen topics of interest to sociolinguistics, including the following:

bilingualism and bidialectalism

code switching

jargons

registers

amount of verbalization in a society

language planning

 

Language and culture

People have often talked about the relationship between language and culture. Some,

such as Sapir and Whorf (early 20th century), hypothesized that language constrains

thought and perception.

Linguistic determinism

Edward Sapir and Benjamin Whorf studied Amerindian languages of North America and

noticed that the lexical and grammatical structures were often quite distinct from English

and other Indo-European languages. Based on their studies they proposed that language

controls a person's thinking processes in some way. In 1929 Sapir said:

Human beings...are very much at the mercy of the particular language which has

become the medium of expression for their society...the 'real world' is to a large

extent unconsciously built upon the language habits of the group. (Quoted in

O'Grady, et. al. 1994:242)

Whorf later stated:

We dissect nature along lines laid down by our native language. The categories

and types that we isolate from the world of phenomena we do not find there

because they stare every observer in the face; on the contrary, the world is

presented in a kaleidoscopic flux of impressions which has to be organized by our

minds—this means largely by the linguistic systems of our minds. (Quoted in

O'Grady, et. al 1994:242)

Eastman notes:

"The views of Sapir and Whorf have been taken to mean that people who speak

different languages segment their world differently: thus, the French language

structures French reality by constraining what French speakers pay attention to,

Swahili does the same for the Swahili 'world'—if there's a word for 'it' in the

language we see 'it,' if not we don't." (Eastman 1990:103)

The concept of language limiting perception has become known as the "Whorfian

Hypothesis" or as "linguistic determinism".

Our concepts structure what we perceive, how we get around in the world and

how we relate to other people. Our conceptual system thus plays a central role in

defining our everyday realities. (Lakoff and Johnson 1980:3)

The individual's whole experience is built upon the plan of his language.

(Henri Delacroix)

 

Linguistic relativity

Many linguists disagree with the strong deterministic stand of Sapir and Whorf. The fact

that people often have ideas for which they have no adequate words indicates that they

use words to label what they see, not that they see what they have words for. Based on

Berlin and Kay's (1967) in-depth study of color terms in a variety of languages, linguists

conclude that frequently used perceptual categories receive names or words, while less

frequently used concepts, while perceived, do not always have separate lexical items.

Thus, as the differences between colors, for example, become important to a society,

more words will be added to distinguish them.

Nida notes:

Whorf regarded language as largely determinative, but most other scholars have

taken the position that the structure of language simply increases the facility with

which people recognize certain distinctions; for example, the matter of color

contrasts, kinship relations, and classifications of fauna and flora. It is certainly

true that language reflects certain aspects of social structure. Relative status, for

example, is marked at least in a general way in a number of European languages

by means of varying forms of second person pronouns...Language is not,

however, a direct inventory of a culture, but rather a kind of index, often based on

what one might call 'fossilized relics' of the past. For the most part, language

follows society rather than determining it. (Nida 1986:11) [emphasis added]

The idea that the grammatical structure of a language reflects the thought

structure of those speaking it and that it correspondingly reflects the differences

from the thought of those speaking a language with different grammatical

structure, has very great difficulties.

(James Barr 1961:39)

Since form and meaning are arbitrary for the most part, it is not helpful to interpret a

culture through its grammar.

What is originality? To see something that has no name as yet and hence cannot

be mentioned although it stares us all in the face. The way men usually are, it

takes a name to make something visible for them. Those with originality have for

the most part also assigned names. (Nietzsche, The Gay Science 1882-7)

The whole problem can be stated quite simply by asking, "Is there a meaning to

music?" My answer would be, "Yes." And "Can you state in so many words what

the meaning is?" My answer to that would be, "No."(Aaron Copland, composer)

 

Language variation

Overview

Each individual speaks in a variety of ways, even within the same 'language' or 'dialect'.

These variations may be due to a number of factors, including:

age Old terms vs. new terms (slang)

geographical region e.g., Texas, New York City, Chicago

occupation Insider talk (jargon)

social situation Formal vs. informal

social status Talking with "equals" vs. talking with "superiors"

The different social settings that affect speech are called domains (at home, at school, at

church, at the office.)

It [is] necessary to acquire the skills needed for communicating in a mixed

society, and…this requires a melting and blending of vernacular and…standard

speech, and a grasp of the occasions in which each, or both, [are] called for.

(Ralph Ellison, Going into the Territory, 1986)

Dialects and languages

Dialects can be defined in terms of geography or various social parameters.

Blair (1990:2) explains the difference between a dialect and a language as follows:

Dialects are "speech varieties which are linguistically similar enough to be

intelligible to speakers of a related variety."

Languages are "speech varieties which are so linguistically dissimilar as to be

unintelligible to speakers of related varieties."

A language is a dialect that has an army and navy. (Max Weinreich, 1894-1969)

 

Dialects can be analyzed in terms of their distinctive sounds, words, or syntax.

Sounds

phonetic differences (e.g., higher or lower vowels)

phonological differences (e.g., no consonant clusters)

Words

difference inflectional rules

different meanings (e.g., a carbonated beverage = soda, pop, soft drink, etc.

Syntax

different word order

variant Phrase-Structure Rules

Standard dialects versus other forms

Linguistic aspects

Descriptive studies of the sounds, words and syntax

Social aspects

Studies of the opinions of people towards different ways of speaking

Studies of how people become "insiders" in a particular group of speakers

Educational aspects

Studies of the effect of particular ways of speaking (or writing) on learning in a

school setting

Studies of government policies towards language variety in school

 

Appropriate language forms

Slang

Slang is "informal, nonstandard vocabulary, usually intelligible only to people from a

particular region or social group" (Crystal 1992:355) and is often seen by outsiders as

crude or impolite. It serves as a way of identifying "insiders".

Jargon

Jargon or argot is "technical terms and expressions used by a group of specialists, which

are not known or understood by the speech community as a whole" (Crystal 1992:200-

201). Jargon is more for ease of communication than for excluding others, although

insiders need to remember that they are using specialized words when they attempt to

communicate with people from other professions.

Gender specific words

Many languages have grammatical distinctions between "gender" (e.g., he vs. she).

Sometimes the "masculine" word is used as a generic and an affix must be attached to

indicate that the noun is feminine (e.g., waiter/waitress, actor/actress).

It has always been lawful, and always will be, to issue words stamped with the

mint-mark of the day. (Horace, Ars Poetica, c. 8 BC)

 

Language attitudes

It is not easy to begin discussing language attitudes without first defining attitudes and

discussing how these attitudes might be measured.

Attitude can be discussed in terms of three components:

thoughts (cognitive)

feelings (affective)

predispositions to action (behavioral or conative)

Advertisers seek to utilize positive language attitudes to affect behavior. Expensive items

are often announced by male speakers with a proper British accent; appeals to young

people are made using more informal language.

"S'pose a man was to come to you and say Polly-voo-franzy—what would you

think?"

"I wouldn' think nuffin; I'd take en bust him over de head..."

"Shucks, it ain't calling you anything. It's only saying, do you know how to talk

French?"

"Well, den, why couldn't he say it?"

(Mark Twain, Huckleberry Finn, XIV)

When we consider the role of language attitudes we need to differentiate the significant

differences that exist in any society.

members of the language group

spokesmen for the ethnic group

mainstream population

educators

policy makers

 

Strategies for measuring language attitudes

Fasold (1984) notes a number of means of measuring language attitudes. These may be

either direct or indirect.

matched-guise tests

A test subject listens to information in two languages or language varieties and is

asked to evaluate the speaker in terms of intelligence, openness, etc. In reality, the

speaker is the same person, who is fluent in both languages.

semantic differential scales

A test subject is asked to evaluate varieties of language on a scale of friendliness.

questionnaires

A test subject responds to a written questionnaire about language varieties. Such

questionnaires might ask which language the subject uses in a given situation or

what the subject feels about people who speak a certain way.

interviews

The test subject is asked to talk about language and the interviewer records the

basic points of the discussion.

observation

The language researcher observes which language is used in which situations,

what people say about language varieties, etc.

Attitudes and group identity

Our identity is related to our attitudes towards our own speech and that of others.