Site hosted by Angelfire.com: Build your free website today!
EEG ADHD Neurofeeback

Applications of EEG – Neurofeedback for Attention Deficit Disorder

T. Druckman, M.Ed. & A. Minevich, B.Sc. (OTR), M.A. (Psyc)

 

Abstract

In the nineties, various treatment modalities have been proposed for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). One such treatment is EEG Neurofeedback. This review cites much of the research, to date, on the application of EEG neurofeedback for ADHD. Key terms necessary for the understanding of ADHD and neurofeedback are discussed. This article also outlines the most commonly used assessment tools, protocols, and electrode placement for conducting neurofeedback research as well as clinical applications for ADHD. Lastly, directions for future research are also discussed.

 

Literature Review

In the nineties, various treatment methods have been proposed for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). One such treatment is Electroencephalographic (EEG) neurofeedback. This article reviews much of the research to date on the application of EEG neurofeedback in the treatment of ADHD. Most commonly used assessment tools, protocols, and electrode placement for conducting neurofeedback research as well as clinical applications for ADHD are explored. In addition, basic terms necessary for the understanding of ADHD and neurofeedback are discussed. Lastly, directions for future research are proposed.

In the early 1970s, researchers first proposed several theories and protocols for using neurofeedback as an assessment and treatment approach for children with ADHD. In 1973, Satterfield, Lesser, Saul and Cantwell proposed a "low-arousal" hypothesis for hyperkinetic children. He found that under-arousal corresponded to decreased amplitudes in high frequency EEG (beta). Seifert & Lubar (1975) published the first article demonstrating a reduction in seizure activity by using neurofeedback training. Lubar & Bahler (1976) published a series of case studies showing the effectiveness of sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) training in reducing seizures (replicating Sterman's 1973 findings). This study found that these seizure patients also experienced increased attentiveness in school. These findings prompted another case study by Lubar and Shouse (1976) to determine the effectiveness of neurofeedback training using SMR to help children with ADHD. This blind crossover study provided the first clear evidence that neurofeedback training, utilizing SMR with theta inhibition, was an effective intervention for working with an ADHD child.

In 1976, Lubar began treating ADHD children using neurofeedback. He noticed that those children with ADHD, with or without hyperactivity, demonstrated a difference in specific types of brain waves. For example, the "beta" brain wave was found to be significantly lower in amplitude and duration compared to "theta" activity, which was higher in amplitude. More specifically, he found those children with attentional and reading difficulties, but not with hyperactivity problems, produced excessive theta activity and deficit beta production. In 1985, Lubar, Bianchini, Calhoun, & Lambert provided a strong rationale involving theta suppression for ADHD children. More extensive studies were published following this study, confirming the relationship between slow wave patterns and ADHD, and providing evidence for the effectiveness of neurofeedback training for ADHD children (e.g., Janzen, Graap, Stephanson, Marshall, & Fitzsimmons, 1995; Linden, Habib, & Radojevic, 1996; Lubar, 1991; Lubar & Lubar, 1984; Lubar & Shouse, 1997; Shouse and Lubar, 1979; Lubar, Swartwood; Swartwood, & O'Donnell, 1995; Lubar, Swartwood, Swartwood, and Timmerman, 1995; Othmer, Kraiser, & Othmer, 1995; Tansey & Bruner, 1983; Tansey, 1984, 1985, 1990). Moreover, recent studies have provided evidence of a relationship between the ability to change EEG and improve attention (Lubar, Swartwood, Swartwood & O'Donnell, 1995; Lubar, Swartwood, Swartwood & Timmerman, 1995).

After several years of research, Lubar et al. (1995) have concluded that for children, under the age of 14, the reduction in theta activity seems to be the key component correlated with reduction of ADHD symptoms. These authors suggest that there is a maturational lag that is reflected in the persistence of excessive theta activity for ADHD subjects when compared to age-matched norms. In addition, Mann et. al., (1992) have supported these findings, concluding that the distribution of EEG frequency for ADHD boys correspond to those of more immature brain activity, exhibited by younger children. For adults, the findings have shown that increasing the amplitude and duration of beta brain wave activity seems to be of primary importance.

From 1986 to 1991, topographic brain mapping studies further clarified the difference in EEG's between ADHD and matched controls. In 1992, Mann, Lubar & Zimmerman, Miller, and Muenchen found that theta activity was obtained in various locations of the brain (e.g., frontal and central) and decreased beta activity was found in frontal and temporal locations. These findings demonstrated that ADHD (with hyperactivity) formed a neurologically distinct group from controls.

Other assessment modalities have also found differences between ADHD and control subjects. Positron Emission Tomography (PET) data indicated decreased glucose metabolism in areas of the brain involved in motor activity and attention in ADHD (hyperactive) versus normal controls (cited in Lubar seminar at the A.A.P.B. fall workshop 1996; Zametkin et al., 1990). Later, Sieg, Gaffney, and Preston (1995) used Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography (SPECT) to explore the brain imaging of ADHD children. These researchers demonstrated that ADHD children exhibit a maturational delay in brain regions associated with attention. These findings are supported by EEG research which also found that the comparison between the brain mapping of ADHD and non-ADHD boys revealed increased theta production and decreased beta production in ADHD boys (Mann, et al., 1992).

Rossiter and LaVaque (1995) compared the effectiveness of neurofeedback to stimulant medication in reducing ADHD symptoms. The neurofeedback subjects exhibited improvements in Test of Variable Attention (TOVA) scores that were comparable to the control group receiving medication. The results indicated that neurofeedback is a viable alternative to the use of stimulant medication. Neurofeedback researchers have indicated that for many ADHD individuals the long-term benefits of neurofeedback outweigh the state-dependent learning effect of medication (Lubar, Swartwood; Swartwood, & O'Donnell, 1995; Lubar, Swartwood, Swartwood, and Timmerman, 1995; Rossiter and LaVaque, 1995).

 

Terminology

This section includes key terms necessary in understanding the process and application of neurofeedback.

ADHD Defined

The DSM-IV criteria for classification of ADHD are grouped into inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Children, adolescents and adults with ADHD, with or without hyperactivity, have difficulty concentrating. They often underperform in school and at work even though they are quite bright. The primary characteristics of ADHD include inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. What distinguishes ADHD children from others is the prevalence, and severity of these symptoms, in a wide range of situations and circumstances.

Neurofeedback and It's Uses for Treating ADHD

In recent years Topographic Brain-Mapping studies have been used to identify regions of the brain, which are associated with increased activity during specific states. Some researchers have classified bandwidths of electrical activity by names such as beta, theta, SMR, EMG, and alpha (see table 1). Classifying bandwidths allows researchers to relate different states to one or more of these bandwidths.

EEG Frequencies

Table 1

beta

Produced during states of concentration and attention activity. Beta is defined differently by various researchers:

Lubar (1991) 16-20 Hz

Othmer & Othmer (1995) 15-18 Hz

Janzen & Fitzsimmons (unpublished manuscript) 12-20 Hz

EMG

Electromyography; muscle generated artifact that, if not isolated, affects EEG readings. For example, large eye or jaw movements can markedly interfere with reading brain wave patterns.

Range of 50-150 Hz

theta

 

 

 

 

 

 

alpha and delta

This slow brainwave is associated with daydreaming or off-task behavior. Theta is defined differently by various researchers:

Lubar (1991) 4-8 Hz

Janzen & Fitzsimmons (unpublished manuscript) 4-8 Hz

Othmer & Othmer (1995) 4-7 Hz

Alpha (8-12 Hz) and Delta (0-4 Hz) are generally not focused on by ADHD-neurofeedback research. In normal development, alpha increases between the ages of 12-14, while theta levels decrease at this time. This maturational stage is delayed in ADHD population throughout the 12-14-age range.

SMR

A neurological state that researchers suggest lowers physical restlessness. The natural ability to increase SMR waves (or inhibit impulsive action or physical activity) in the ADHD population is markedly lower. SMR is associated with EEG activity in the following ranges:

Lubar (1991) 12-15 Hz

Othmer & Othmer (1995)12-15 Hz

Sterman (1996 ) 12-20 Hz, with peak activity between 12-14 Hz

Janzen & Fitzsimmons (unpublished manuscript ) 12-16 Hz

What happens during Neurofeedback training for ADHD?

Sensors are attached to the scalp and ear using conductive paste. These sensors measure brain activity, which is then converted into frequencies and amplitudes produced by the brain. The computer then converts this information into visual and auditory feedback. Through this feedback, the person can begin to experience how their brain is reacting during different situations, and learn how to change their brain wave patterns. By continuously practicing neurofeedback techniques, the individual learns to change and control their brain patterns. During neurofeedback training, ADHD individuals are taught to increase their fast wave activity (e.g., beta and SMR) and decrease their slow wave activity (e.g., theta).

Assessment Tools (pre-post)

These assessment tools have been utilized by various studies in ADHD research:

Sites and Locations

Some researchers use "Scull Caps" to collect data. This device is similar to a swimming cap. The cap, which contains 19 electrodes, fits over an individual's head. Each electrode is placed upon one cite identified in Figure 1. Research has shown that certain areas of the brain exhibit more activity associated with attention (see Table 2). Some researchers use a "bi-polar" placement. This method involves placing two electrodes over specific sites (brain regions). Others use "mono-polar" placement, which involves using one electrode over one brain region and one electrode attached to the ear. Both have been shown to be efficient methods of conducting neurofeedback.

Figure 1 Cite Placement

LEGEND

F = Frontal T = Temporal O = Occipital

C = Central P = Parietal Z = Middle

odd numbers = Left hemisphere

even numbers = Right hemisphere

Cite Locations and Protocols

Table 2

Application

Reference Placement

Bipolar Placement

Length of Treatment

Protocol

ADD

CPZ age 7-9

CZ age 10-15

FCZ age 16-25

FZ age 26-50

C3 and C4 all ages

CZ-PZ

age 7-9

FCZ-CPZ

age 10-15

FZ-CZ

age 16+

30-50 + sessions

inc. 16-20 Hz

dec 4- 8 Hz

inc. 16-20 Hz

dec 6-10 Hz

inc. 16-22 Hz

dec 6-10 Hz

inc. 16-24 Hz

dec 6-10 Hz

ADHD

CZ, C4

or C3

all ages

C1-C5

all ages

20-30 sessions then continue with

ADD protocol

inc. 12-15 Hz

dec 4-8 Hz

or 6-10 Hz based on age

Source: Fifth annual winter conference on Brain Function-EEG, modification and training; advanced meeting colloquium, (Palm Springs February 1997)

Subjects and Training Sessions

 

ADHD neurofeedback training has been applied to both children and adults, ranging between 20-50 sessions of approximately 40-50 minutes. In addition, some studies have incorporated an educational component with feedback. Table 4 represents various research paradigms used in neurofeedback training for ADHD. It is important to note that some studies have not controlled for factors such as location of sites and training times.

Table 4

Ages

Sessions & times/week

Feedback & Education

Sources

5-15

40 sessions

45 minutes

twice per week

Feedback and education

Linden et al., 1996

8-21

40 sessions (3-5 wk)

50 min

20 min feedback

Neurofeedback only

Rossiter & LaVaque, 1995

child & adult

20-40 sessions

45 min

Neurofeedback only

Othmer, Kraiser

and Othmer, 1995

10-19

2 sessions per week

40 min session

Feedback alone or combined with education

Lubar and Lubar , 1984

8-19

40 sessions

50 minutes

predetermined order

Feedback , listening & reading

Lubar, Swartwood, Swartwood and O'Donnell, 1995

Conclusions and Directions for Future Research

Despite only a few decades of scientific research, neurofeedback is quickly growing into a mature science. In particular, the application of neurofeedback has become recognized as a valuable tool in the treatment of ADHD. This growth has been aided by factors such as advancement of technology and better understanding of ADHD. However, since most early studies that examined the efficacy of neurofeedback as a treatment for ADHD were composed of small sample sizes, conclusions are difficult extrapolate. Hence, future research must utilize even greater sample sizes and employ more standardized methodology. In addition, subject selection should also be more rigorous. The use of appropriate control groups and the recruitment of pure ADHD without comorbidity of other disorders are necessary. Moreover, when selecting subjects, maturational shifts should be taken into account. Furthermore, methodological differences should be drawn between pure neurofeedback versus neurofeedback and academic training. Lastly, long term follow-up studies are necessary to track the efficacy of neurofeedback.

In conclusion, neurofeedback offers numerous possibilities for the future. The application toward human cognition and peak performance are enormous. The ability to control our neural responses promises to allow individuals to have a better quality of life. In particular, the application of neurofeedback appears to be a viable alternative to medication as a treatment for ADHD.

References

 

Janzen, T. & Fitzsimmons, G. Theta, An electrophysiological marker of attention deficit. Unpublished Manuscript.

Janzen, T., Graap, K., Stephanson, S., Marshall, W., & Fitzsimmons, G. (1995). Differences in baseline EEG measures for ADD and normally achieving preadolescent males. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 20, 65-82.

Linden, M., Habib, T., & Radojevic, V. (1996). A controlled study of the effects of EEG biofeedback on cognition and behavior of children with attention deficit disorders and learning disabilities. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 21, 35-49.

Lubar, J. (1991). Discourse on the development of EEG diagnostics and biofeedback for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 16, 201-225.

Lubar, J. & Lubar, J. (1984). Electroencephalographic biofeedback of SMR and beta for treatment of deficit disorders in a clinical setting. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 9, 1-23.

Lubar, J. & Shouse, M. (1976). EEG and behavioral changes in a hyperactive child concurrent with training of sensorimotor rhythm (SMR). A preliminary report. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 1, 293-306.

Lubar, J. F. & Bahler, W.W. ( 1976). Behavior management of epileptic seizures following EEG biofeedback training of the sensorimotor rhythm, Biofeedback and Self Regulation, 1(1), 77-104.

Lubar, J. F. & Shouse, M. N. (1977). Use of biofeedback in the treatment of seizure disorders and hyperactivity. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds), Advances in clinical child psychology. New York, Plenum Press. 203-265.

Lubar, J. F., Bianchini, B. A., Calhoun, W. H., & Lambert, E. W. (1985). Spectral analysis of EEG differences between children with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 18, 403-408.

Lubar, J. F., Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N., & O'Donnell, P. H. (1995). Evaluation of the effectiveness EEG neurofeedback training for ADHD in a clinical setting as measured by changes in T.O.V.A. scores, behavioral ratings, and WISC-R performance. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 20(1), 83-99.

Lubar, J.F., Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N., & Timmerman, D. L. (1995). Quantitative EEG and auditory event-related potentials in the evaluation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, effects of methylphenidate and implications for neurofeedback training. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 143-160.

Mann, C., Lubar, L. F., Zimmerman, A. W., Miller, C. A., & Muenchen, R. A. (1992). Quantitative analysis of EEG in boys with Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), A controlled study with clinical implications. Pediatric Neurology, 8, 30-36.

Othmer, S. & Othmer, S. F. (1995). EEG biofeedback for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Available from EEG Spectrum, Inc., 16100 Ventura Blvd., Encino, CA 91436.

Othmer, S., Kraiser, D. A., & Othmer, S. F. (1995). EEG biofeedback training for attention deficit disorder, A review of clinical findings using the T.O.V.A. as a measure. Available from EEG Spectrum, Inc., 16100 Ventura Blvd., Encino, CA 91436.

Rossiter, T. & LaVaque, T. J. (1995). A comparison of EEG biofeedback and psychostimulants in treating attention deficit hyperactivity disorders. Journal of Neurotherapy, 48-59.

Satterfield, J. H., Lesser, L. I., Saul, R. E., & Cantwell, D. P. (1973). EEG aspects in the diagnosis and treatment of minimal brain dysfunction. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 205, 274-282.

Schwartz, M. S., & Associates. (1995). Biofeedback, A practitioner's guide (2nd edition). The Guilford Press. New York, 493-522.

Seifert, A. R. & Lubar, J. F. (1975). Reduction of epileptic seizures through EEG biofeedback training. Biological Psychology, 3, 157-184.

Shouse, M. N. & Lubar, J. S. (1979). Operant conditioning of EEG rhythms and ritalin in the treatment of hyperkinesis. Biofeedback and Self Regulation, 4 (4), 299-311.

Sieg, K. G., Gaffney, G. R., Preston, D. F., & Hellings, J. A. (1995). SPECT brain imaging abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Nuclear Medicine, 20(1), 55-60.

Sterman, M. B. (1973). Neurophysiologic and clinical studies of sensorimotor EEG biofeedback training, some effects on epilepsy. Seminars in Psychiatry, v5 (n4), 507-525.

Sterman, M. B. (1996). Physiological origins and functional correlates of EEG rhythmic activities, Implications for self-regulation. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 21 (1), 3-33.

Tansey, M. (1985). Brainwave signatures - An index reflective of the brains functional neuroanatomy, Further findings on the effects of EEG/SMR biofeedback training on the neurologic precursors of learning disabilities. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 85-89.

Tansey, M. (1984). EEG sensorimotor rhythm biofeedback training, Some effects on the neurologic precursors of learning disabilities. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 1, 163-177.

 

Tansey, M. & Bruner, R. (1983). EMG and EEG biofeedback training in the treatment of a 10 year old hyperactive boy with a developmental reading disorder. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 8, 25-37.

Tansey, M. (1990). Righting the rhythms of reason, EEG biofeedback training as a therapeutic modality in a clinical office setting. Medical Psychotherapy, 3, 57-68.

Zametkin, A. J., Nordahl, T. E., Gross, M., King, A. C., Semple, W. E., Rumsey, J., Hamburger, S., & Cohen, R. M. (1990). Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with hyperactivity of childhood onset. New England Journal of Medicine, 323, 20, 1361-1366.

 

To E-Mail us:

Tamara Druckman rivers@sympatico.ca

Alexander Minevich aminevich@writeme.com

Email: aminevich@yahoo.com